1) The muck heaps at farms one, two, three and four were covered

1). The muck heaps at farms one, two, three and four were covered from early March 2009

during the seasonal vector-free period ( European Commission, 2007), until the end of May 2009 following the spring peak in Culicoides emergence ( Sanders et al., 2011). Farming activities on the farms prevented the muck heaps from remaining covered for a longer time period. The muck heaps at the remaining four farms (farms five, six, seven and eight, Fig. 2) remained uncovered throughout and GDC-0199 price were used as controls, to allow an assessment of the overall trend in Culicoides subgenus Avaritia populations for the 2009 season when compared to previous seasons (2006–2008). Light suction traps were located within 100 m of muck heaps, livestock housing and grazing pasture at all farms and greater than 5 km from the muck heaps of any neighbouring farms. Although seasonal variation in the number of livestock located in close proximity (<100 m)

to the light suction Akt inhibitor trap did occur within farms, this variation was consistent between years and was primarily associated with the variation between livestock being housed during winter and grazed at pasture during spring, summer and autumn. To assess the effect of covering muck heaps on the first generational peak in ‘local’ adult populations of the Culicoides subgenus Avaritia, trap catches were analysed using generalised linear models assuming Poisson errors and a log link function. Furthermore, the models included overdispersion (to allow for the high week-to-week variability in catches), temporal autocorrelation amongst the observations (to allow for dependence between observations) and hierarchical

structure in the model parameters (to allow for between-farm differences) ( Sanders et al., 2011). The number of female subgenus Avaritia Culicoides collected (yjk) at the jth observation on farm k (collected ADP ribosylation factor on day tjk) was assumed to follow a Poisson distribution, that is, equation(1) yjk∼Poisson(μjk),yjk∼Poisson(μjk),with the expected trap catch, μjk, given by, equation(2) log(μjk)=cjkb0k(C)+b1k(C)sin2π365(tjk−ϕjk)+(1−cjk)b0k(U)+b1k(U)sin2π365(tjk−ϕjk)+σjk+εjk.Here, cjk indicates whether (cjk = 1) or not (cjk = 0) the muck heap was covered, the terms including sine functions describe seasonality in the Culicoides population when the muck heap is covered (C) or uncovered (U) (here b0 is the log mean population, b1 is the amplitude and ϕ is the phase), while σjk allows for overdispersion in the data and ɛjk allows for temporal autocorrelation between observations. Between-site variation was incorporated by assuming the parameters for each site are drawn from higher-level distributions, so that, bik(•)∼N(μbi(•),σbi(•)2),ϕk=182.5+182.5ϕ′k, ϕ′k∼Beta(aϕ,bϕ),while overdispersion in the data was modelled as, σjk∼N(0,σd2).Finally, temporal autocorrelation was described by a stationary AR(p) process ( Diggle et al.

The species dependence is further evidenced by differences in cor

The species dependence is further evidenced by differences in cortical organization between cat and mouse V1. First, in the cat V1, simple cells are found in thalamocortical recipient layers (layer 4 and 6; Hirsch and

Martinez, 2006) and the spatial arrangement of feed-forward thalamic inputs is important for the establishment of OS. In the mouse V1, neurons in layer 4 are mostly monocontrast and simple cells primarily appear in layer 2/3 (Liu et al., 2009). OS of simple cells we recorded likely results from BTK inhibitor ic50 integrating recurrent inputs from layer 2/3 and feed-forward inputs from layer 4 (Dantzker and Callaway, 2000 and Mooser et al., 2004), some of which may already be orientation tuned (Niell and Stryker, 2008 and Ma et al., 2010). Second, there is a columnar organization of OS in the cat V1, whereas in the mouse V1 neurons preferring different orientations are intermingled in a “salt-and-pepper” fashion (Ohki et al., 2005). It has been shown that patterns of synaptic inputs to cat V1 neurons Epacadostat mouse vary depending on the cell’s location in the orientation map (Schummers et al., 2002 and Mariño et al., 2005). Within the orientation domain, the neuron receives intracortical inputs from other cells sharing the same orientation preference, whereas at pinwheel centers the intracortical inputs are from cells with a wide range of different orientation preferences. Therefore, both excitation

and inhibition are much more broadly tuned at pinwheel centers than within orientation domains (Mariño et al., Florfenicol 2005; note that the simple or complex cell type was not explicitly identified in this study). In comparison, synaptic inputs to our recorded cells exhibited less selectivity than those to cat cells within orientation

domains. This observation is consistent with a lack of orientation maps in the mouse V1 and the result that local synaptic inputs to the layer 2/3 neuron have a wide variety of orientation preferences (Jia et al., 2010). A more careful comparison indicates that excitatory inputs to mouse simple cells are more selective than those to cat cells at pinwheel centers, implying that synaptic connections to mouse simple cells might be slightly more selective than cat cells at pinwheel centers. Our study demonstrates two effects that significantly impact OS. The first one is the membrane blurring of selectivity when PSP responses are generated from excitatory inputs alone. This is due to a saturation of PSP response with increasing excitatory input strength, caused by a rapid reduction in the excitatory driving force as the depolarizing potential approaches the reversal potential for excitatory currents. For relatively small excitatory conductances, the membrane blurring would be less a problem, since the initial phase of the input-output curve can be approximated by a linear function crossing the origin, i.e., f(x) = ax.

Oral clonidine has resulted in high serum levels in breastfed inf

Oral clonidine has resulted in high serum levels in breastfed infants (http://toxnet.nlm.nih.gov/). 1. Antihypertensive drug therapy may be used to keep sBP at 130–155 mmHg and dBP at 80–105 mmHg (I-B; Low/Weak). 1. For women with comorbid conditions, antihypertensive drug therapy should be used to keep sBP at <140 mmHg and dBP at <90 mmHg (III-C; Low/Weak). Management of non-severe pregnancy hypertension is much debated. Any antihypertensive therapy will, compared with placebo or no therapy: decrease transient severe hypertension

(RR selleck inhibitor 0.50; 95% CI 0.41–0.61) without a difference in other outcomes, including preeclampsia or preterm delivery [243]. However, antihypertensive lowering of BP may reduce fetal growth velocity [61], [247] and [248]); not all subsequently published data are consistent with this [344]. The definitive CHIPS Paclitaxel (Control of Hypertension In Pregnancy Study) RCT addressing the issue of BP targets in non-severe hypertension will publish its results in 2014 [345]. No reliable long-term developmental outcome data exist [346] and [347] (see Effect

on long-term child development). Women without comorbid conditions should receive antihypertensives to lower dBP to 80–105 mmHg, recognizing that non-severe hypertension is not an absolute indication for treatment outside pregnancy [7]. The upper dBP acknowledges BP variability, BP measurement inaccuracies, and the desire to avoid a dBP ⩾ 110 mmHg. The lower dBP reflects concern around limiting uteroplacental perfusion [247] and [248], and recommendations outside pregnancy [7]. In contrast, women with comorbid conditions (Table 1) should probably have their BP lowered to <140/90 mmHg. Lower limits for BP goals are unclear. Outside pregnancy, Bay 11-7085 <130/80 mmHg is specified only with diabetes mellitus but to achieve risk reduction over a longer timeframe [7] and [348]. CHEP recommendations provide initial guidance about treatment of secondary causes of hypertension [7]. There is little to guide the choice of antihypertensives in women with or without

co-morbidities. Many antihypertensives have been compared with placebo or no therapy: methyldopa, labetalol, other pure beta-blockers (acebutolol, mepindolol, metoprolol, pindolol, and propranolol), calcium channel blockers (isradipine, nicardipine, nifedipine, and verapamil), hydralazine, prazosin, and ketanserin [246]; ketanserin, isradipine, nicardipine, and mepindolol are not used in Canada. In comparative trials (usually of beta-blockers vs. methyldopa), beta-blockers (i.e., labetalol, pindolol, metoprolol, or oxprenolol) were more effective antihypertensives than methyldopa (RR 0.75; 95% CI 0.58–0.94), without other differences in outcomes [246] and [349] (see ‘Aspects of care specific to women with pre-existing hypertension’ and ‘Effects on long-term child development’). Be familiar with a number of antihypertensive options.

Importantly, in our electrophysiological study, kif17−/− slices s

Importantly, in our electrophysiological study, kif17−/− slices showed a deficit in L-LTP ( Figure 5I), a neuronal EGFR cancer event mimicking the in vivo process of memory consolidation. Thus, the impaired long-term memory of fear conditioning in kif17−/− mice may reflect an effect not only on the acquisition of memory, but also on processes related to consolidation. Consistently, further in vitro (

Figures 7A–7C) and in vivo (Figures 7D–7K and 8C–8F) experiments identified activity-dependent transcriptional control of the genes encoding KIF17 and NR2B by CREB. This could be the molecular basis underlying the impairment in the consolidation of long-term memory in kif17−/− mice. Based on these findings, it is considered that kif17−/− mice have defects in multiple phases of long-term memory formation. The level of KIF17/NR2B is increased in response to synaptic inputs. This process is mediated by CREB activation. A functional relationship between selleck chemicals llc KIF17/NR2B and CREB is suggested on the basis of the following evidence: (1) CREB activation was impaired in kif17−/− mouse neurons ( Figures 7A–7C). (2) Training for spatial memory formation upregulated KIF17, NR2B/2A, and pCREB in the hippocampus ( Figures 7D–7K). (3) Absence

of KIF17 blocked the upregulation of pCREB, NR2B, and NR2A induced by spatial memory tasks ( Figures 7D–7K). (4) Expression of a dominant-negative form of CREB suppressed kif17 and nr2b transcription and decreased the amount of NR2B ( Figures 8C–8F). (5) Expression of constitutively active CREB increased the levels of kif17 and nr2b transcription and increased the levels of KIF17 and NR2B. The NR2A level was also increased without alteration of nr2a transcription ( Figures 8C–8F). Our previous work provides further supporting evidence, namely, (6) the upstream promoter regions of kif17 and nr2b contain CREB binding sites,

and (7) overexpression of KIF17 resulted in an increase in the level of pCREB, concomitant with enhanced spatial memory ( Wong et al., nearly 2002). Taking all of these findings into account, it is suggested that the reciprocal enhancement of KIF17-mediated transport and CREB phosphorylation works in concert to fine-tune synaptic plasticity and thereby is important in memory formation (Figure 8G). In this scheme, KIF17 acts not only as a mediator for constitutive receptor transport, but also as a key player in the “augmented NR supply during memory formation” (Figure 8G). In brief, when synapses are activated, transcription of kif17 and nr2b is induced by CREB, resulting in an increase in the levels of KIF17 and NR2B. This increase leads to increased transport and further synaptic accumulation of NR2B. Conversely, in kif17−/− neurons, failure of the reciprocal regulation between KIF17/NR2B and CREB, as well as the accelerated degradation of NR2A mediated by the ubiquitin-proteasome system, could contribute to the memory disturbances found in kif17−/− mice.

, 1978; Schiller, 1992) Recently, however, there is growing evid

, 1978; Schiller, 1992). Recently, however, there is growing evidence that, in the inner retina, crosstalk between the On and Off pathways generated via crossover circuits can change the receptive field properties of retinal

ganglion cells (Demb and Singer, 2012; Münch et al., 2009). To test whether crosstalk contributes to the reversal that we see, we blocked the On pathway using selleck screening library an mGluR6 agonist, L-AP4 (5–20 μM), that blocks input from photoreceptors to On bipolar cells (Slaughter and Miller, 1981). As expected, in the presence of L-AP4 (n = 24), all DSGCs showed no On response to stationary spots. The majority of these cells exhibited Off responses that were directionally tuned toward posterior directions (75%, 18/24 cells; Figure 5A), as previously described (Kittila and Massey, 1995). The remaining cells (25%, 6/24) were classified as non-DS. However, three of the non-DS cells displayed Off responses that were tuned to both posterior and anterior directions, making these cells axial selective rather than direction selective (Figure 5B; Figures S6A and S6B). In addition, four of the directionally tuned cells also presented a response toward

both directions, but the responses toward the ND were significantly smaller than the responses toward the PD. Interestingly, in these axial-selective cells, the timing of the response relative Selleck SB203580 to stimulation in the ND was different than the timing relative to stimulation in the PD (Figure S6C, Isotretinoin top). This implies that before adaptation, the delayed Off response to stimulation

in the original ND is masked by the On pathway. Hence, crosstalk between the On and Off pathways must normally contribute to the On-Off DSGC’s directional preference. Presenting the adaptation protocol to direction-selective and axial-selective cells (n = 21) in the presence of L-AP4 led to several changes in their responses to visual stimulation. First, a significant percentage of cells stopped responding to gratings (29%, 6/21), indicating that without On pathway signaling, a subset of cells loses its response to stimulation in the original PD and does not gain a new PD response. Second, cells that continued to respond to gratings showed reduced directional tuning (mean DSI decreased from 0.54 ± 0.23 to 0.18 ± 0.63), with 20% (3 out of 15) exhibiting a reversed PD (Figures 5A and 5B). Interestingly, the response timing relative to the stimulus resembled the timing relative to the stimulus when ND stimulation was given to axial-selective cells before adaptation (Figure S6C), indicating that the circuit mediating the ND response before adaptation in L-AP4 is identical to the circuit mediating the reversed response after adaptation. Third, after adaptation, 40% (6/15 cells) of the direction-selective and axial-selective cells exhibited an On response to a spot test (Figure 5A; Figures S6A and S6B).

8 ± 0 05 Hz) Starting with P5 short episodes (0 2 ± 0 003 s, n =

8 ± 0.05 Hz). Starting with P5 short episodes (0.2 ± 0.003 s, n = 1951 events from 19 pups) of low gamma-band (37.08 ± 0.15 Hz) oscillations overlaid spindle-shaped oscillations with main frequency of 9.2 ± 0.11 Hz and large amplitudes (251.61 ± 2.82 μV). Due to the tight connection between the superimposed gamma oscillations and the slow theta-alpha bursts, we defined this pattern of prefrontal activity as nested gamma spindle bursts (NG) (Figures 1B,

1Cii, and 1Ciii). They occurred at a frequency of 0.67 ± 0.09 bursts/min, lasted 2.12 ± 0.03 s and were accompanied (Figure 1Ciii) or not (Figure 1Cii) by MUA. Although the main difference between SB and NG was the presence of superimposed gamma episodes, the two patterns of prefrontal activity have also distinct properties (Figure S2). Similar to early urethane-independent oscillations in the primary sensory cortices prefrontal SB and NG were marginally modified by progressive reduction BIBW2992 mouse of the urethane dose from 1 to 0.125 g/kg body weight (n = 16 pups). Their occurrence and main frequency remained constant, whereas their amplitude decreased from 145 ± 7 μV to 107.7 ± 5.8 μV for Lumacaftor mouse SB and from 277.7 ± 10.1 μV to 160 ± 8 μV for NG. With ongoing maturation the properties of SB and NG modified significantly. Their occurrence, duration, amplitude, and dominant

frequency gradually increased with age (Figure 1E). Around P10–11 the PFC switched from discontinuous SB and NG to continuous oscillatory rhythms (Figures 1D and S4), suggesting that the neuronal networks generating oscillatory patterns underwent a substantial process of reorganization. The continuous rhythm with main frequency in theta-band (6.11 ± 0.03 Hz, n = 19 pups) and amplitudes ranging from 56 to 544 μV expressed

superimposed short episodes of gamma activity (Figure 1D). The amplitude and the dominant frequency of continuous oscillatory activity in the PFC were GBA3 relatively stable during the second postnatal week (Figure 1E). These results indicate that corresponding to the previously reported delayed anatomical maturation of the PFC, network activity emerges here later than in the V1 or S1 of age-matched rat pups. The presence of discontinuous and later of continuous theta-gamma rhythms mirrors early complex intra- and intercortical interactions. To gain a first insight into the network interactions leading to the generation of oscillatory patterns in the neonatal PFC we analyzed the relationship between neuronal discharge and SB/NG (Figure 2A). The mean firing rate during the whole recording was very low (0.67 ± 0.11 Hz, n = 7 pups), MUA predominantly accompanying the prefrontal oscillations. Only a low fraction of oscillation-associated spikes (15.7% ± 2.1%, n = 2479 spikes from 6 pups) was organized in bursts. When calculating separately the firing rate for SB and NG, a significantly (p < 0.001) higher spike discharge was associated with NG (14.

Other theoretical frameworks stress the role of the hippocampus i

Other theoretical frameworks stress the role of the hippocampus in spatial processing in general (Burgess et al., 2002 and O’Keefe and Nadel, 1978), a role that could be extended to perceptual judgments on scenes. LY294002 nmr It has also been argued that the hippocampus is necessary in perceptual tasks that

require binding of information (Warren et al., 2012). These ideas have been challenged by studies failing to find scene perception impairments in patients with hippocampal damage (Hartley et al., 2007, Kim et al., 2011 and Shrager et al., 2006). The current study suggests that the distinction between state- and strength- based perception can help to reconcile the conflict in the literature. In previous studies (Aly and Yonelinas, 2012), we found strong evidence that strength-based perception is affected by manipulations of global featural relationships, whereas state-based perception is disproportionately driven by detection of relatively local, item-level differences. For example, when the only difference between a pair of scenes was a specific feature (e.g., a window in one scene that is absent in the other), perceptual

decisions were based primarily on state-based perception. In contrast, when the featural relations within the scenes differed from one another, performance relied more heavily on strength-based perception. Moreover, individuals reported identifying specific, local details when responses were state-based, and generalized feelings of overall difference/sameness when responses were strength based. In the current fMRI study, the hippocampus

and parahippocampal cortex see more were sensitive to strength-based perception, but, importantly, we also found that other regions of the brain were sensitive to state-based perception. For example, the posterior parietal cortex exhibited state-based, but not strength-based, effects (M.A., C.R., and A.P.Y., unpublished data). Viewed in the context of our previous studies, the present results suggest significant constraints on when and how the hippocampus would be expected to contribute to perception. We propose that the hippocampus is involved in perceptual discriminations that require a representation Oxymatrine of relational or conjunctive information. Not only did the hippocampus track the perceived “strength” of perceptual change, the more basic finding of hippocampal adaptation (greater activation for “different” than “same” trials) suggests the hippocampus forms precise representations of visual scenes. The differences we introduced were subtle—on a given trial, the paired scenes are essentially identical with very small distortions. Thus, finding hippocampal adaptation for such small visual differences provides further evidence that the hippocampus represents precise relational information (Bakker et al., 2008 and Lacy et al., 2011). Because state-based perception plays a larger role in performance when perceptual manipulations involve discrete features (e.g.

g , creatine-targeting Ckb and 17-AAG-targeting

g., creatine-targeting Ckb and 17-AAG-targeting buy DAPT Hsp90s) (Herbst and Wanker, 2007 and Dorsey and Shoulson, 2012). In summary, the red module contains proteins that are highly correlated with Htt (including Htt itself) and is enriched in a highly connected group of proteins involved in proteostasis,

14-3-3 signaling, microtubule-based transport, and mitochondria function. The second most Htt-correlated module is the blue module, with its member proteins enriched in the cortex and playing roles in presynaptic function. The most significant GO terms enriched in blue module are “Coated Membrane” and “Neurotransmitter Transport” (Table S14). The top IPA Canonical Signaling Pathways enriched in blue module are “GABA Receptor Signaling,” “Clathrin-Mediated Endocytosis,” and “Huntington’s Disease Signaling.” The hub proteins in blue module (Ap2a2, Dnm1, and Syt1) are members of a Htt protein network previously established based on ex vivo interactions with mHtt fragments and are validated as genetic modifiers in an HD fly model (Kaltenbach et al., 2007). Together, this evidence

supports the notion that the blue module contains cortex-enriched Htt interactors that preferentially function in presynaptic terminals and hence may influence corticostriatal neurotransmission that is known to be affected in HD (Raymond et al., 2011). The pink module is a cerebellum-enriched module with HD-relevant RAD001 manufacturer hub proteins functioning in calcium signaling (Itpr1 and Itpr2), mitochondria function (Ndufa9, Ndufs2, and Uqcrc2), and glutamate receptor function (Grid2 and Slc1a3). Not surprisingly,

several hub proteins are either selectively expressed (Grid2 and Slc1a3) or highly enriched in the cerebellum (Itpr1, Syt2, and Gpd1; see Allen Brain Atlas). Consistent with the idea that cerebellar-enriched Htt interactors may confer neuroprotective function, one interesting pink module protein, Ucqrc2, was shown to be one of nine core modulators of the proteostasis network (e.g., mHtt polyQ fragment and endogenous metastable proteins) in a genome-wide Caenorhabditis elegans screen ( Silva et al., 2011). Since our interactome also identified more Ucqrc2 peptides in brain tissues (cerebellum) and at ages (2 months) relatively unaffected Non-specific serine/threonine protein kinase in HD mice ( Table S7), this evidence strongly encourages further investigation in the role of Ucqrc2 and its interaction with Htt in HD selective pathogenesis. The yellow module is driven by top hub proteins involved in excitatory postsynaptic function (Table S14). Two of the top hub proteins (Grin2b/NR2b and Dlg4/PSD95) have been implicated in pathogenesis in HD mice (Zeron et al., 2002 and Fan et al., 2009). Another interesting member, beta-catenin (Ctnnb1), is a known modifier of mHtt-induced toxicity in HD cell and fly models (Godin et al., 2010 and Dupont et al., 2012).

, 2007, Gao et al , 2008 and Mank et al , 2008) Behavioral assay

, 2007, Gao et al., 2008 and Mank et al., 2008). Behavioral assays have been developed that are amenable to simultaneous

neuronal monitoring and a complete anatomical wiring diagram of the visual system appears within reach ( Seelig click here et al., 2010, Maimon et al., 2010 and Chklovskii et al., 2010). Taking advantage of these tools, two groups describe their first results concerning the mapping of the Reichardt model onto neuronal hardware. The minimal circuitry that is thought to be involved in motion detection consists of photoreceptors in the retina, which synapse onto two types of large monopolar cells called L1 and L2 in the next neuropil, the lamina. These cells project in turn onto neurons in the medulla called Mi1 and Tm1 that contact T4 and T5 cells before reaching large tangential cells in the lobula plate that are well characterized and known to represent the output of the Reichardt model ( Figure 1C). The starting point of the first article, by Eichner and colleagues (2011) (this issue of Neuron), is the recognition that multiplication see more over the

entire range of negative and positive brightness fluctuations, as required by the Reichardt model, is unlikely to be achieved by single neurons. This led to the proposal that brightness changes be initially half-wave rectified and then multiplied, which should be much easier to implement in single neurons. That is, multiplication would be carried out on signals that are clipped at zero, sON(t) = max(0, s(t)) and sOFF(t) = max(−s(t),0), resulting in four distinct subbranches of the Reichardt model: ON-ON, ADAMTS5 ON-OFF, OFF-ON, and OFF-OFF, respectively (Figure 1B of Eichner et al., 2011). Indeed, since this formulation is equivalent

to the original model, a wealth of experimental data supports it (e.g., Figure 2 of Eichner et al., 2011). Yet, the tangential cell recordings reported by Eichner and colleagues suggest that half-wave rectification of fast brightness fluctuations is not the only signal driving the Reichardt detector: quite remarkably, brightness changes occurring up to 10 s earlier in the first stimulated channel still impact changes in the second one (their Figure 3). Clark et al. (2011) (discussed below) essentially confirms this result at the behavioral level (their Figure 6D). This leads Eichner and colleagues (2011) to formulate a model that includes these much slower changes, or “DC” components (terminology borrowed from electrical engineering; their Figure 4A). As a byproduct, two of the four subbranches of the original implementation, the ON-OFF and the OFF-ON, can be entirely disposed of, while still reproducing a wide range of experimental data.

Notably, the density of structures double positive for either pre

Notably, the density of structures double positive for either presynaptic active zone marker (munc13-1, bassoon) and postsynaptic scaffolding proteins (homer, PSD95) was unaltered, indicating that mSYD1A loss in cultured neurons does not change synapse density but only presynaptic composition (Figure 2F). We tested whether function of mSYD1A is specifically required

in the presynaptic cell by introducing a human, siRNA-resistant form of SYD1A (hSYD1A) into the synaptophysin-mCherry-positive cells. Importantly, this was sufficient to rescue the presynaptic terminal Apoptosis Compound Library supplier density back to wild-type level (Figures 2C and 2D). Recording of miniature excitatory postsynaptic currents (mEPSCs) in mSYD1A knockdown cultures further supported a presynaptic phenotype. The mEPSC frequency in knockdown neurons was reduced by 43% ± 7% as compared to PLX-4720 cost controls (Figure 2G). This reduction was rescued by re-introduction of hSYD1A using lentiviral infection (see Figure S2 for re-expression level and further controls for the RNA interference experiments). In combination with the morphological effects on synaptic vesicle distribution these results demonstrate that mSYD1A controls presynaptic differentiation in cultured neurons and is required in the presynaptic cell. Some functions of invertebrate

SYD-1 proteins are thought to rely on a catalytically inactive Rho-GAP-like domain whereas others have been pinpointed to the PDZ-domain of the protein (Hallam et al., 2002 and Owald et al., 2012). Mammalian SYD1 proteins differ significantly from their invertebrate counterparts in that they lack PDZ-domains and contain Rho-GAP domains that may be active based on amino acid sequence analysis all (Figure S3A). We directly probed GAP activity of mSYD1A in intact cells using a FRET-based assay (Itoh et al., 2002 and Pertz et al., 2006; Figure 3A). Using a RhoA sensor,

we observed significant RhoA inactivation in cells expressing mSYD1A. The degree of RhoA inactivation was similar to that observed for p50rhoGAP, a well-characterized GAP (Figures 3B and 3C). Importantly, mutation of the arginine finger (Graham et al., 1999) in mSYD1A (R436A) strongly reduced mSYD1A activity observed in this assay and no change in FRET was observed when Lin-2/CASK, a protein lacking GAP domains, was introduced (Figures 3B and 3C). Similarly, the amino acid alterations from the Rho-GAP consensus seen in the C. elegans and Drosophila SYD-1 proteins strongly reduce activity toward RhoA ( Figure S3B). Finally, we used morphological changes of neuronal dendrites as a read-out for RhoA regulation in cerebellar granule cells. Overexpression of C-terminally Myc-tagged mSYD1A but not the R436A or ΔYRL mutants led to a significant increase in dendritic trees compared to GFP-transfected neurons ( Figure S3D).